How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (1)

Link to Publisher's site

PLoS One. 2022; 17(6): e0270457.

Published online 2022 Jun 30. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0270457

PMCID: PMC9246182

PMID: 35771844

Masako Katsuyama, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing,How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (2)1,* Tomomi Narita, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology,1 Masaya Nakashima, Investigation,1 Kentaro Kusaba, Investigation,1 Masatoshi Ochiai, Data curation, Investigation,1 Naomi Kunizawa, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation,1 Akihiro Kawaraya, Methodology, Writing – original draft,2 Yukari Kuwahara, Methodology,2 Masahiro Horiuchi, Methodology, Writing – original draft,3 and Koji Nakamoto, Methodology3

Nobuyuki Sakai, Editor

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement

Abstract

The gas emanating from human skin is known to vary depending on one’s physical condition and diet. Thus, skin gas has been gaining substantial scholarly attention as an effective noninvasive biomarker for understanding different physical conditions. This study focuses on the relationship between psychological stress and skin gas, which has remained unclear to date. It has been deduced that when participants were subjected to interviews confirmed as stressful by physiological indicators, their skin emitted an odor similar to stir-fried leeks containing allyl mercaptan and dimethyl trisulfide. This characteristic, recognizable odor appeared reproducibly during the stress-inducing situations. Furthermore, the study deduced that individuals who perceive this stress odor experience subjective tension, confusion, and fatigue (Profile of Mood States scale). Thus, the study findings indicate the possibility of human nonverbal communication through odor, which could enhance our understanding of human interaction.

Introduction

Human emotions can be conveyed in gestures, vocal pitch, facial expressions, and other forms, and people can often perceive and understand these nonverbal indicators. The study focuses on the effects of body odor as a form of nonverbal communication.

Scholars have reported two main routes for the generation of body odor. The first is degeneration by oxidation that occurs through the interaction between microorganisms and sebum/sweat on the skin’s surface [1,2]. The second is odor generated through the skin from inside the body. This type of odor is described as transcutaneous blood volatile organic compounds or skin gas [35]. For a long time, scholars have recognized that body odor can change along with changes in physical condition, such as during illness. Recent studies have proposed that skin gas can be analyzed to determine the state of the body along with other biological indicators, such as saliva, urine, and blood [6]. Since then, research in this field has advanced significantly.

For example, in the medical field, it has been shown that diabetic patients have a higher acetone content in their skin gas than healthy individuals [7,8]. In addition to this finding, interventions have been pursued to manage various states of the disease. Similarly, many studies reported that dogs can detect the presence of early-stage cancer in their owners through odor. As a result, dogs are being trained for cancer detection [9,10]. Alternatively, we investigated the relationship between skin gas and the effects of diet, physical condition, and aging in healthy volunteers instead of people suffering from illnesses or disorders. The results indicated that older people emit 2-nonenal [11], and people with constipation emit high levels of p-cresol [12].

Based on these findings, skin gas is deemed a beneficial means of investigating the body’s condition, including diseases and general physical health. The study further hypothesized that psychological changes may alter skin gas and ultimately impact others. Therefore, the study investigated changes in the odor of skin gas during stress-inducing sessions, with the objective of identifying key components, and ultimately, confirming the impact on others.

Our findings indicate that humans reproducibly emit discernible odors through the skin containing allyl mercaptan and dimethyl trisulfide during interviews that induce psychological tension. Furthermore, we found that when other people smell this odor, they undergo subjective tension, confusion, and tiredness. According to the results of these studies, skin gas may contain ingredients that convey human psychological conditions.

Materials and methods

Effects of tension on skin gases

Subject

The study subjects consisted of 40 healthy Japanese women aged 35–44 years. As the sampling skin gas in this study was performed on the hands, it was necessary to avoid tobacco odor contaminants found on the hands of smokers; hence, the target population was nonsmokers. In addition, as body odor is known to differ between men and women and with age, the study was initially restricted to women. The subjects underwent skin-gas sampling as well as saliva sampling and electrocardiography.

In another odor assessment study conducted to confirm that the applicability of this phenomenon transcended the female Japanese subjects to include four healthy, nonsmoking Japanese men in their 20s and 30s, three Chinese men and women, and two French men. The subjects were told about the purpose, methods, anticipated clinical benefits, and disadvantages of the study before it started. All participants provided written informed consent, and the study complied with the ethical principles established by the Declaration of Helsinki. Furthermore, the study received the approval of the Shiseido Ethics Committee (Approval Number: C01402, C01555, C01577, C01824).

Participants in the study were instructed (a) not to consume odorous food one day before the study, (b) avoid contact with any strongly smelling objects, and lastly, (c) not to wear any fragrance.

Test methods and skin-gas collection methods

Exam outline: In a room kept under a constant temperature and humidity, the test was conducted twice on different days: the first time with the task of answering questions from an unknown interviewer for 20 min and the second time on a day of relaxing in a chair and reading a magazine. On day one, subjects were fitted with an ECG monitor before the interview, and their electrocardiographic activity was measured continuously. Their nondominant hand was kept in a skin-gas sampling bag during the interview and while they were reading a magazine. Their saliva was collected three times. The first collection was taken as a baseline while resting at home during the same time frame as of the interview but on a different day. The second collection was taken after the interview, and the third one was after the subjects read a magazine (Fig 1).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (3)

Effects of tension on skin gases.

Skin-gas samples were collected from each subject in two situations: under stress (answering questions for 20 min from an interviewer whom they did not know) and at rest (sitting on a chair and relaxing while reading a magazine on a different day). Subjects washed their hands with unscented soap before sampling. The electrocardiographic activity was measured continuously by an ECG monitor (Ledar Circ, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharmaceutical). Saliva samples were collected three times. The first measurement refers to samples collected as a baseline while the subjects were at home resting during the same time frame as that of the interview but on a different day. The second sample was collected after the interview, and the third one was collected after the subjects had read a magazine.

Under the skin-gas sampling procedure, participants washed their hands with unscented soap and their nondominant hand was covered with a sampling bag made of polyvinyl fluoride resin film (Tedlar®) with a one-way stopco*ck attached. First, the stopco*ck was opened, and excess air was pushed out of the collection bag in which the hand was placed. Next, a bag (500 mL) filled with nitrogen gas was connected via a short silicone tube, and the nitrogen-filled bag was pressed to move the nitrogen into the collection bag. The empty nitrogen bag was then removed from the stopco*ck. After a certain amount of time (Fig 2), a storage bag (ANALYTIC-BARRIER™, GL Sciences Corporation) was connected via a short silicone tube to the stopco*ck. The collected gas was transferred from the collection bag to the storage bag by pressing the collection bag. This gas was used as the sample.

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (4)

Collection bag for skin gas emanating from subjects’ hands.

Hands washed with unscented soap were covered with sampling bags made from a fluorinated ethylene-propylene copolymer material. The air inside was replaced with nitrogen and recovered after a certain period.

Physiological index measurements

Autonomic status. Four electrodes were placed under the left and right subclavian bones and the tenth left and right ribs. The electrocardiographic activity was measured continuously by an electrocardiogram monitor (Ledar Circ, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharmaceutical).

Based on the electrocardiogram data, a power spectrum analysis was done on the heartbeat “R wave” intervals using autonomic nervous system activity analysis software (Sumitomo Dainippon Pharmaceutical, FLUCLET®). The intensities of the high frequency (HF) (ranging from 0.2 to 2 Hz) and low frequency (LF) (ranging from 0.04 to 0.15 Hz) components were determined. Additionally, the LF/HF component was used as an activity index of the sympathetic nervous system.

Salivary cortisol measurements. Their saliva was collected three times. The first collection was done as a baseline while the subjects were resting at home during the same time frame as that of the interview but on a different day. The second collection was done after the interview, and the third one was done after the subjects read a magazine. We observed this process to avoid cortisol changes resulting from circadian rhythms, which are particularly variable in the morning. Salivary cortisol was quantified using ELISA (Cortisol Salivary Immunoassay Kit Salimetrics LLC).

Odor assessment. A panel of four odor experts sniffed and recorded the characteristics of skin gases collected during stress and at rest respectively. The samples were labeled by number and the examiners made their assessments without knowing the context in which the samples were taken. A five-point Likert-type scale (0 = Not smelly at all, 1 = Slightly smelly, 2 = Smelly, 3 = Distinctly smelly, 4 = Very strongly smelly) evaluated the intensity of the characteristic smell. The results revealed that all gas samples taken during the interview had a characteristic odor, like that of stir-fried leek, while samples taken at baseline did not have any perceivable odor. The skin-gas samples taken during a tense state were subjected to the following analysis for component identification of the odor resembling stir-fried leek.

Identification of characteristic odor components

In order to identify the main components of the characteristic odor from the gas samples collected during a tense moment found by the sensory evaluation, a component analysis was performed in the following order.

1) Analysis 1

Preparation of standard sample

Dimethyl trisulfide (Sigma-Aldrich) was diluted to 0.05% (w/v) with dichloromethane.

Samples

A total of 1 L of sampled gas (taken during the interviews) was collected for analysis. Several gas samples were integrated by aspirating them to a 6 L canister, the inner surface of which had been deactivated and prepared with negative pressure.

Gas sampling from canister to Cooled Injection System CIS 4

The 6 L canister (ENTECH INSTRUMENTS, INC., Simi Valley, CA, USA) was connected to Cooled Injection System CIS 4 (GERSTEL GmbH & Co. KG, Mulheim an der Ruhr, Germany) using a carrier gas line T-connected with an inert tube. CIS4 with a glass-wool liner was cooled to −150°C during sampling; a 165 mL calculated gas sample was trapped from a canister.

Gas chromatography-mass spectrometry/olfactometry (GC-MS/O) analysis

The GC6890N/5973 inert system (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) was equipped with CIS4 (GERSTEL) and an olfactory detection port ODP3 (GERSTEL). The odor was identified by one odor expert and one GC analyst who had learned the odor characteristics from the odor expert. The GC-MS/O was fitted with a DB-WAX column (60 m x 0.25 mm i.d., 0.25 μm of film thickness, Agilent J & W). The column temperature program was set as follows: the initial temperature was kept at 40°C for 5 min and then increased to 200°C by 5°C/min. Helium was used as the carrier gas at a constant linear velocity of 32 cm/s. The CIS4 temperature was programmed as follows: the initial temperature was kept at −150°C for 0.01 min and then increased to 220°C by 12°C/s. The ion source temperature was set at 230°C. Injections were conducted in a solvent vent mode. The ion source was operated at 70 eV using electron ionization (EI), and the mass spectrometer was set in SCAN mode.

2) Analysis 2

Preparation of standard gas samples

Allyl mercaptan (Wako Pure Chemical Industries, Ltd., Osaka, Japan) was diluted with isopropyl myristate (Nikko Chemical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) to a concentration of 1 ppm and 5 ppm. 1 μL of each concentration was put into a 1 L sampling bag (Smart Bag PA, GL Sciences Inc.) filled with 400 mL of nitrogen and used as standard gas samples after equilibrating for over six hours at room temperature before the experiment. The final concentrations of the standard gas samples prepared from each of the 1 and 5 ppm solutions were ca. 2.5 ng/L and ca. 12.5 ng/L, respectively.

Dynamic headspace sampling

Each sampling bag was connected to an air sampling pump JAS-15M II (Japan Analytical Industry Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) with a silicone tube. An adsorption cartridge mini-PAT (Japan Analytical Industry Co., Ltd.), composed of a sample tube filled with Tenax GR wrapped around foil, was connected between the bag and the air pump. By sucking 400 mL of the gas at 200 mL/min using the pump, odorants in each sample were collected onto the adsorbent. The adsorbed odorants were desorbed at 280°C by curie point injector JCI-55 (Japan Analytical Industry Co., Ltd.) and then subjected to GC-O analysis.

Solid-phase microextraction (SPME) sampling

An SPME fiber coated with 85 μm carboxen/polydimethylsiloxane (CAR-PDMS) (Supelco Inc., Bellefonte, PA, USA) was used. The fiber was exposed to each sample gas for 30 min at room temperature. The adsorbed odorants were then analyzed using GC-MS.

Gas chromatography-olfactometry (GC-O) analysis

A GC-2010 plus (Shimadzu Co., Kyoto, Japan) was equipped with an olfactory detection port ODP275 (GL Sciences Inc., Tokyo, Japan). The odor was confirmed by two odor experts and a GC analyst. The GC was fitted with a DB-Sulfur SCD column (60 m x 0.32 mm i.d., 4.2 μm of film thickness, Agilent J & W). The column temperature program was set as follows: the initial temperature was kept at 35°C for 3 min and then increased to 250°C by 10°C/min. Helium was used as the carrier gas at a constant linear velocity of 31 cm/s. The injector temperature was set at 230°C. Injections were conducted in a splitless mode.

Gas Chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) analysis

The GC-MS analyses were performed using an Agilent GC7890A/5975C system (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) equipped with a DB-Sulfur SCD column (60 m x 0.32 mm i.d., 4.2 μm of film thickness, Agilent J & W). The column temperature program was set as follows: the initial temperature was kept at 35°C for 3 min and then increased to 250°C by 10°C/min. Helium was used as the carrier gas at a constant linear velocity of 31 cm/s. The injector and the ion source temperature were set at 230°C. Injections were conducted in a splitless mode. The ion source was operated at 70 eV using EI, and the mass spectrometer was set in the selected ion monitoring (SIM) mode. The mass to be analyzed for allyl mercaptan was m/z74.

Subjective reaction when smelling the model odor

The study developed a model odor using the components identified in the previous section and examined their psychological impact on other people.

Samples of model odor

Allyl mercaptan (AM) and dimethyl trisulfide (DMTS) were diluted with isopropyl alcohol to create five levels of concentration (AM: DMTS = [0.5: 2.5 ppb], [1: 5 ppb], [2: 10 ppb], [5: 25 ppb], [10: 50 ppb]). 10 μL of each concentration was applied to a small piece of cotton and the subjects sniffed it.

Subjects for subjective reaction test

The subjects consisted of 33 Japanese women aged 35–44 years. All participants were informed of the purpose, methods, anticipated clinical benefits, and disadvantages of the study before it started. We obtained written informed consent from all participants, and the study complied with ethical principles established by the Declaration of Helsinki. Furthermore, the study received the approval of the Shiseido Ethics Committee (Approval Number: C01555, C01577). To minimize the effects of food and other odors on the subjective effects of smell, the subjects were instructed (a) not to consume odorous food one day before the study and (b) avoid contact with any strongly smelling objects. They were also instructed not to wear any fragrance.

Assessing the subjective state

The study first confirmed the per-subject threshold before assessing their subjective states. A model tension-stress odor whose concentration was adjusted to the odor threshold of each subject was used as the test sample. The subjective state was assessed before and after sniffing the odor using the Profile of Mood States (short version, POMS 2), 2nd edition. POMS 2 contains 35 questions to assess the following moods: anger–hostility (AH), confusion–bewilderment (CB), depression–dejection (DD), fatigue–inertia (FI), tension–anxiety (TA), vigor–activity (VA), and friendliness (F). POMS 2 is a self-reporting questionnaire for rapid assessment of short-term or sustained emotional states, and widely used not only for patients but also for healthy subjects. The higher the T-scores for AH, CB, DD, FI, and TA, the more negative the mood. Conversely, positive mood states, such as VA and F, indicate that the higher the T score, the better the mood [1315].

Before taking measurements, participants were instructed to relax and stay comfortably seated for at least 10 min. They then filled out the POMS 2. Next, each participant inhaled their threshold concentration of the tension-stress odor applied to a cotton and placed under their nose for 2 min. Immediately following this, the participants repeated the POMS 2 test. POMS was answered on a 5-point Likert-style scale and calculated as a T score using standard POMS 2 calculation methods for seven mood status assessments.

Statistical analysis

The results of the test samples and each experiment are presented as means ± SDs. Bonferroni’s method was used for the significance tests for the sympathetic nervous system activity and for the results of the salivary cortisol levels. The relationship between odor intensity and autonomic nervous activity was assessed using pairwise nonparametric correlations (Spearman’s). Finally, Wilcoxon’s signed-rank test determined significant differences in the results of the psychological index. A p-value <0.05 on both sides was considered statistically significant. All statistical analyses were performed with EZR (Saitama Medical Center, Jichi Medical University, Saitama, Japan, version 1.54), a graphical user interface for R (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).

Results

Physiological responses under emotional tension

To examine the changes in skin-gas odor resulting from psychological distress, the study investigated physiological response changes in the components of skin gas gathered from the subjects’ hands during the interview sessions that purposefully induced stress. For one physiological response, the heart rate “R wave” intervals were calculated using electrocardiogram data. The HF intensity levels (0.2 to 2 Hz range) and LF (0.04 to 0.15 Hz range) components were identified. The LF/HF components were calculated using the activity index of the sympathetic nervous system.

The LF/HF during the interview was significantly higher than those of previous and subsequent measurements, which indicates that the sympathetic nervous system was dominant (**p < 0.01 after Bonferroni correction) (Fig 3).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (5)

Autonomic nervous system activity during a stress-inducing interview.

The R wave intervals for the heart rate were calculated using electrocardiogram data. The high frequency intensity levels (HF, 0.2 to 2 Hz range) and low frequency (LF, 0.04 to 0.15 Hz range) components were identified. The LF/HF components were calculated using the activity index of the sympathetic nervous system. The LF/HF values collected during the interview were significantly higher than that of the previous and subsequent measurements, which indicates that the sympathetic nervous system was dominant (**p < 0.01 after Bonferroni correction).

Salivary cortisol levels after the interview were significantly higher than those at baseline were (*p < 0.05), and while subjects were reading (**p < 0.01, after Bonferroni correction) (Fig 4).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (6)

Salivary cortisol measurements.

Saliva was collected three times. The first sample was collected within the same time frame as that of the interview but on a different day while the subjects were at home resting. The second sample was collected after the interview, and the third one was collected after the subjects had read a magazine. Salivary cortisol levels after the interview were significantly higher than those at baseline (*p < 0.05) and those while the subjects were reading (**p < 0.01, after Bonferroni correction).

Sensory assessment of skin-gas samples gathered under emotional tension

Four expert odor raters sniffed the skin-gas samples taken during stress and at rest, respectively. When they found a perceived odor, they rated and recorded its characteristics and intensity on a 5-point scale ranging as 0 = Not smelly at all, 1 = Slightly smelly, 2 = Smelly, 3 = Distinctly smelly, 4 = Very strongly smelly. Samples were numbered and evaluated by the rater without prior knowledge of the conditions under which the sample was collected. Thus, four odor experts recorded the presence of a unique odor similar to stir-fried leeks common to multiple samples, all of which had been collected at the time of the stress-inducing interviews.

Additionally, the intensity of this characteristic stir-fried leek-like odor was positively correlated with the rate of increase in sympathetic nerve activity during the interview (Spearman’s correlation coefficient, r = 0.66; p <0.01; Fig 5). To confirm that this phenomenon was not limited to the female Japanese subjects; various other participants (Japanese: four males, Chinese: one female, two males, French: two males) were tested while they experienced mental tension. Thus, the same characteristic odor was detected from all participants while they were experiencing mental tension. On the other hand, this stir-fried leek-like odor was not detected at all in samples taken during cycling runs, conducted to cause an increase in heart rate due to physical exercise instead of psychological factors (Fig 6).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (7)

Relationship between the intensity of tension-stress odor and autonomic nervous activity during the interview.

A panel of four odor experts evaluated the collected skin-gas samples to compare differences between those emanated during stress and at baseline. From all interview samples, we discovered a characteristic odor resembling stir-fried leek. The intensity of this characteristic odor was positively correlated with the rate of increase in sympathetic nerve activity during the interview compared with before the interview (Spearman’s correlation coefficient: r = 0.66 (p < 0.01)).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (8)

Relationship between the intensity of tension-stress odor during exercise and changes in heart rate.

A panel of four scent experts evaluated the collected skin-gas samples to identify odor differences among individuals exercising and at rest. No characteristic odor resembling stir-fried leek was observed in any samples when the heart rate increased because of exercise.

Identifying characteristic odorants

GC-O analysis investigated the main components contributing to the characteristic stir-fried leek-like odor of skin gas collected during the stress interviews. The results revealed that there were a few odor-active components similar to the scent.

1) Characteristic component 1

A sample collected during the stress interview showed a peak close to the characteristic odor with a retention time (RT) = 20.571 minutes. (Fig 7). Moreover, the standard DMTS was detected at RT = 20.571 min. (Fig 7A and 7D). At the peak of RT = 20.571 min., these ions were detected by extracting mass chromatograms with m/s = 126 and 79 as a feature of DMTS (Fig 7B and 7C). Hexanol and nonanal were detected before and after the peak at RT = 20.571. From the correlation (R2 = 0.9984) between the RTs and the retention index (RI) of the three components registered in the MS private library database, DMTS was identified as the target substance (Fig 8). Furthermore, using GC-MS/O analysis, we determined that the peak of the RT = 20.571 is DMTS from the quality of its odor.

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (9)

Results of analysis GC-O.

A sample during a tension interview showed a peak that was close to the characteristic odor with a retention time (RT) = 20.571 minutes. Moreover, standard DMTS was detected at RT = 20.571 minutes. (a, d). At the peak of RT = 20.571 min, these ions were detected by extracting mass chromatograms with m/s = 126 and 79 as a feature of DMTS (b, c).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (10)

Mass pattern of DMTS and correlations RT and RI.

Hexanol and nonanal were detected around the peak of RT = 0.20.571. The correlation between these RTs and the coefficient of determination (RI) of the three components registered in the MS Private Library Database of Takata Koryo Co., Ltd. was R2 = 0.9984 (b).

2) Characteristic component 2

One of the few odor-active components similar to the tension-stress odor had a more intense sulfurous and alliaceous scent than the others. From its odor description and GC RT, we estimated it to be AM. To confirm the presence of AM in the skin gas, GC-MS (SIM) analyses were performed. On SIM chromatograms of the skin gas, a peak in each skin gas sample was detected at the same RT as AM, and the peak intensity of the skin gas was higher than that of 2.5 ng/L and lower than that of 12.5 ng/L (Fig 9).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (11)

Selected ion monitoring (SIM) chromatograms according to allyl mercaptan (AM) (m/z74).

The peak in each skin-gas sample collected during the stress interview (b, c) was detected at the same RT as that of AM, and the peak intensity of the skin-gas was higher than 2.5 ng/L and lower than 12.5 ng/L. (a): The control skin-gas without the characteristic odor representative of skin-gas with the characteristic odor; (b),(c): collected during the stress interview (panels #1 and #2), (d): Nitrogen gas containing AM (2.5 ng/L); (e): Nitrogen gas containing AM (12.5 ng/L).

In subsequent GC-O analyses, RT and odor quality of the characteristic odor detected in skin gas were the same as those in AM. The odor intensity of the skin gas was slightly stronger than the 2.5 ng/L standard sample was, which was consistent with their peak intensity in GC-MS analysis. From these results, the target component was identified as AM.

Subjective effects of the tension odor

The model tension odor, formulated according to the threshold, was continuously sniffed by participants for 2 min. POMS 2 measured the participants’ subjective status at baseline and immediately after sniffing the sample.

The study observed that negative scores of “tension–anxiety” (p < 0.01), “confusion–bewilderment” (p < 0.01), and “fatigue–inertia” (p < 0.01) significantly increased after sniffing the model tension odor (Wilcoxon’s signed-rank test) (Fig 10).

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (12)

Subjective effects of the tension odor.

The model tension odor, formulated according to the threshold, was continuously sniffed by participants for 2 min. POMS 2 measured the participants’ subjective status at baseline and immediately after sniffing the sample. Note: AH: anger–hostility, CB: confusion–bewilderment, DD: depression–dejection, FI: fatigue–inertia, TA: tension–anxiety, VA: vigor–activity, and F: friendliness. The negative scores of “tension–anxiety” (p < 0.01), “confusion–bewilderment” (p < 0.01), and “fatigue–inertia” (p < 0.01) significantly increased after sniffing the model tension odor (Wilcoxon’s signed-rank test).

Discussion

Scholars recently noted that humans release a wide variety of gases from the skin, whose components may act as a barometer for indicating the state of the body. For example, diabetics characteristically release acetone, whereas those with liver disease typically emit ammonia. Moreover, the study confirmed that the human skin releases specific components due to changes in the body unrelated to illnesses, such as aging and constipation. Alternatively, studies have reported body odor in samples collected mainly from the armpit, which is caused by various emotional changes. The majority of studies have examined the reactions of humans or dogs after smelling different states of human odors [1618]. Reports of human odors estimated by statistical methods from gas chromatography data do not indicate how such odors affect human emotions [19]. Therefore, this study hypothesized that changes in the psychological state chemically affect skin gas and its components affect human psychology. Our purpose was to identify the main components of skin gas released during certain emotional states and examine their psychological effects. Among the various psychological changes present, this study chose to use the Trier social stress test method, an easy-to-control method to induce mild tension [20,21].

Collecting body odor from humans can be done in various ways, from the armpits using cotton balls [1618], clothing [2,22], and the back and neck [23]. However, samples taken using such methods contain a mixture of bacterial metabolites of sebum and apocrine gland secretions in addition to the odor that comes directly from the body. In this study, we wanted to see the exact relationship between the changes in the body and the skin gas. For this reason, skin-gas samples were collected from the hand because it contains fewer sebaceous glands and no apocrine glands [24].

During the tension-inducing interviews, autonomic nervous system activity was measured before, during, and after to confirm the subject’s condition. The results indicated that the LF/HF values during the interviews were significantly higher than those before and after, which indicates sympathetic dominance. Salivary cortisol levels after the interview were also significantly higher than those at baseline and those while the subjects were reading were.

The test times were either morning or evening, depending on participants’ availability. It was expected that the influence of the interview could be difficult to observe because of the circadian rhythm of cortisol [25,26], especially in the morning test. Therefore, control saliva was collected at the same time on another day. As a result, at the time of the interview, the stress level was higher in participants than under normal condition.

In addition, a blind sensory evaluation was conducted to ascertain any changes in odor in the skin gas caused by tension and stress. The results revealed that despite the short duration (20 min), all skin-gas samples collected during the interview had a very characteristic odor, similar to stir-fried leeks. Notably, the intensity of this characteristic odor correlated positively with the rate of increase in sympathetic activity during the interviews. In another test, this phenomenon was also observed in men and nonJapanese individuals while in a state of tension. These findings suggest that a characteristic odor is emitted more strongly from humans with greater emotional tension, and this phenomenon may be an instinctive human physiological reaction. In contrast, as a comparison, the results of the sensory evaluation of skin-gas samples taken during cycling exercise (>140 bpm for 20 min), meant to increase subjects’ heart rate, did not identify any specific odor (Fig 6). This result suggests that a mere increase in heart rate and the accompanying increase in thermal sweating was not the direct cause of the characteristic odor released by the skin. In other words, it indicates that the odor is released as a result of psychological change.

We were able to recognize the characteristic odor through sensory evaluation. This odor can even be recognized by the general public, not just odor experts. However, the concentration of the target compound was so low that it was extremely difficult to determine it through gas chromatography. Thus, the study looked for effective sample preparation methods and analytical conditions and finally succeeded in identifying the target compounds, namely, dimethyl trisulfide and allyl mercaptan.

The characteristic odor we were seeking, emanating from the skin during stress-induced interviews, was similar to that of allyl mercaptan; however, it could not be perfectly reproduced on its own, and when the odor of dimethyl trisulfide was added, the smell was much closer to the actual smell we found that was similar to that of stir-fried leeks.

The successful identification of this component makes it possible to artificially reproduce a model odor of tension-stress. In this study, the psychological (subjective) effects of this odor were investigated: a short version of POMS2 was used to assess the subjective changes before and after 2 min of sniffing this model odor at a threshold intensity. The results revealed that smelling the model tension-stress odor increased negative emotions such as tension–anxiety (TA), fatigue–inertia (FI) and confusion-confusion (CB). However, the subjective ratings in this study were provided on a Likert-style scale, and thus, there is a possibility of an anchor effect, where the rater’s memory of the previous score influences the next rating.

Nevertheless, the trends in the results suggest that odors emanating from emotionally stressed individuals may have a psychological impact on those in their vicinity.

In summary, our initial hypothesis that the psychological state of tension and stress chemically affects skin gas and its components affect human emotions has been confirmed beyond the barriers such as substance identification.

This discovery has shown several possibilities. First, the fact that the palm, which does not contain apocrine glands, releases the recognizable substances during psychological changes has opened up possibilities in biogas research. Second, this means that deodorants and fragrances can play a significant role in human communication, not etiquette alone. We are developing a practical fragrance material that does not cheat this odor with a strong scent, but harmonizes it with a weak scent and removes the odor’s negative psychological effects. Third, in another area, the tension-stress odor may be used to identify a tense and stressful psychological state. Thus, this odor could be used to manage mental health conditions and facilitate smooth communication during social interactions.

Moreover, human body odor, in the most extreme cases, can provide us with clues to the location of missing victims during rescue operations or natural/manmade disasters [27,28]. In this area of rescue, the recognition of human odors induced by tension and stress may be beneficial in detecting humans in crisis.

Conclusion

This study demonstrates that psychological changes (tension-stress) influence the components of skin gas. The main components of these substances were also identified and the effect of this odor on subjectivity was clarified. Our findings could lead to a variety of applications, including biogas, deodorants, and life-saving operations. However, several points about this phenomenon require clarification, such as the mechanism of odor generation due to psychological changes, and thus, further research is necessary.

Supporting information

S1 Data

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The research was conducted through collaboration among many individuals across various fields. I am deeply grateful and indebted to all of them.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

References

1. Kanda F, Yagi E, f*ckuda M, Nakajima K, Ohta T, Nakata O. Elucidation of chemical compounds responsible for foot malodor. Br. J. Dermatol. 1990;122:771–776. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2133.1990.tb06265.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Haze S, Gozu Y, Nakamura S, Kohno Y, Sawano K, Ohta H, et al. 2-nonenal newly found in human body odor tends to increase with aging. J. Invest. Dermatol. 2001;116:520–524. doi: 10.1046/j.0022-202x.2001.01287.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Tsuda T, Ohkuwa T, Itoh H. Findings of skin gases and their possibilities in healthcare monitoring. Gas. Biol. Res. Clin. Pract. 2011;125–132. [Google Scholar]

4. Iitani K, Hayakawa Y, Toma K, Arakawa T, Mitsubayashi K. Switchable sniff-cam (gas-imaging system) based on redox reactions of alcohol dehydrogenase for ethanol and acetaldehyde in exhaled breath. Talanta. 2019;197:249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.talanta.2018.12.070 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Arakawa T, Aota T, Iitani K, Toma K, Iwasaki Y, Mitsubayashi K. Skin ethanol gas measurement system with a biochemical gas sensor and gas concentrator toward monitoring of blood volatile compounds. Talanta. 2020;219:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.talanta.2020.121187 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Broza YY, Zuri L, Haick H. Combined volatolomics for monitoring of human body chemistry. Sci. Rep. 2014;4:1–6. doi: 10.1038/srep04611 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Yamane N, Tsuda T, Nose K, Yamamoto A, Ishiguro H, Kondo T. Relationship between skin acetone and blood h-hydroxybutyrate concentrations in diabetes. Clin. Chim. Acta. 2006;365: 325–329. doi: 10.1016/j.cca.2005.09.016 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Nose K, Mizuno T, Yamane N, Kondo T, Ohtani H, Araki S, et al. Identification of ammonia in gas emanated from human skin and its correlation with that in blood. Anal. Sci. 2005;21:1471–1474. doi: 10.2116/analsci.21.1471 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Boedeker E, Friedel G, Walles T. Sniffer dogs as part of a bimodal bionic research approach to develop a lung cancer screening. Interact. Cardiovasc. Thorac. Surg. 2012;14:511–515. doi: 10.1093/icvts/ivr070 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Willis CM, Britton LE, Swindells MA, Jones EM, Kemp AE, Muirhead NL, et al. Invasive melanoma in vivo can be distinguished from basal cell carcinoma, benign naevi and healthy skin by canine olfaction: A proof-of-principle study of differential volatile organic compound emission. Br. J. Dermatol. 2016;175:1020–1029. doi: 10.1111/bjd.14887 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Katsuyama M, Hisada Y. Effects of oral administration of CoenzymeQ10 to nonenal in skin gas of elderly women. Jpn. J. Comple. Alt. Med. 2017;14:17–22. [Google Scholar]

12. Katsuyama M.Characteristic body odors in skin gases generated from the body: Aging odor, constipation odor, and their countermeasures. Aroma Res.2019;20:24–29. [Google Scholar]

13. Heuchert JP, McNair DM, Yokoyama K, Watanabe K. The manual of POMS2 in Japanese 156. Tokyo: Kanekoshobo; 2015. [Google Scholar]

14. Matsumoto C, Nanke K, Furumura S, Arimatsu M, f*ckuyama M, Maeda H. Effects of disposable bath and towel bath on the transition of resident skin bacteria, water content of the stratum corneum, and relaxation. Am. J. Infect. Control. 2019;47:811–815. doi: 10.1016/j.ajic.2018.12.008 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Higashikawa F, Kanno K, Ogata A, Sugiyama M. Reduction of fatigue and anger-hostility by the oral administration of 5-aminolevulinic acid phosphate: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel study. Sci. Rep.2020;29:16004–16011. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Albrecht J, Demmel M, Schöpf V, Kleemann AM, Kopietz R, May J, et al. Smelling chemosensory signals of males in anxious versus nonanxious condition increases state anxiety of female subjects. Chem. Senses. 2011;36:19–27. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq087 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Chen D, Haviland-Jones J. Human olfactory communication of emotion. Percept. Mot. Skills. 2000;91:771–781. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.771 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Rubin D, Botanov Y, Hajcak G, Mujica-Parodi LR. Second-hand stress: Inhalation of stress sweat enhances neural response to neutral faces. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2012;7:208–212. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq097 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Tsukuda M, Nishiyama Y, Kawai S, Okumura Y. Identifying stress markers in skin gases by analysing gas collected from subjects undergoing the Trier social stress test and performing statistical analysis. J. Breath Res. 2019;13:036003. doi: 10.1088/1752-7163/ab0903 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28:76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

21. Mohammadi A, Emamgoli A, Shirinkalam M, Meftahi GH, Yagoobi K, Hatef B. The persistent effect of acute psychosocial stress on heart rate variability. Egypt Heart J. 2019;71:3–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Rikowski A, Grammer K. Human body odor, symmetry and attractiveness. Proc. Biol. Sci. 1999:266:869–874. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0717 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Kimura K, Sekine Y, Furukawa S, Takahashi M, Oikawa, D. Measurement of 2-nonenal and diacetyl emanating from human skinsurface employing passive flux sampler—GCMS system. J. Chromatogr. B. 2016;1028:181–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Nose K, Mizuno T, Yamane N, Kondo T, Ohtani H, Araki S, et al. Identification of ammonia in gas emanated from human skin and its correlation with that in blood. Anal. Sci. 2005;21:1471–1474. doi: 10.2116/analsci.21.1471 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Fang L, Yu Q, Yin F, Yu J, Zhang Y, Zhang Y, et al. Combined cortisol and melatonin measurements with detailed parameter analysis can assess the circadian rhythms in bipolar disorder patients. Brain Behav. 2021;11:e02186. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2186 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Lovallo WR, Whitsett TL, Al’Absi M, Sung BH, Vincent AS, Wilson MF. Caffeine stimulation of cortisol secretion across the waking hours in relation to caffeine intake levels. Psychosom. Med. 2005;67:734–739. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000181270.20036.06 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. Vautz W, Slodzynski R, Hariharan C, Seifert L, Nolte J, Fobbe R, et al. Detection of metabolites of trapped humans using ion mobility spectrometry coupled with gas chromatography. Anal. Chem. 2013;85:2135–2142. doi: 10.1021/ac302752f [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Mochalski P, Ruzsanyi V, Wiesenhofer H, Mayhew CA. Instrumental sensing of trace volatiles-a new promising tool for detecting the presence of entrapped or hidden people. J. Breath Res. 2018;12:27107. doi: 10.1088/1752-7163/aa9769 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

  • PLoS One. 2022; 17(6): e0270457.
  • »
  • Decision Letter 0

2022; 17(6): e0270457.

Published online 2022 Jun 30. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0270457.r001

Nobuyuki Sakai, Academic Editor

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

9 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-36189How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on othersPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. KATSUYAMA,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.We really appreciate for your submitting an interesting report to PLOS ONE. Both reviewers and the editor have found many interesting and important points in this report, and want to read this report in PLOS ONE. However, both reviewers have also founds some problems that should be corrected before acceptance. Many of those problems seem to be minor, but the editor estimated that you want some time to respond the reviewers' comments because of your daily businesses. You can resubmit your manuscript in anytime after you have finished the correction. We are waiting for your resubmission.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 25 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office atgro.solp@enosolp. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Nobuyuki Sakai, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1:No

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1:No

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1:This is a very interesting paper which describes the change in skin odor under phycological stress, identification of skin gases causing the tension-stress odor and emotional effect of the odor to people by sniffing. The human skin gas has been inviting considerable attentions as a source of body odor, mosquito attractant, non-invasive medical biomarkers and so on. While a previous study showed the dermal emission of ammonia increased with physical and/or phycological stress, this study also found new skin gases causing the characteristic odor similar to sulfur-containing compounds. So, I believe this study potentially contains very valuable outcomes which merit publication. However, this manuscript has been poorly written with less objectivity. Please revise the manuscript addressing following points.

P5, L76: I do not have any criticisms on this point, but simply want to know why the study subjects were focused on nonsmoking Japanese woman? Are there any effect of smoking on the human skin gas? Why woman only?

P6, L94: I felt difficulty to catch the sentence that “Afterward, gas obtained from the sampling bags was recovered, placed in a storage bag (ANALYTIC-BARRIER™, GL Science Co., Ltd.) using connected silicone tubes.” Does this mean “Afterward, the sampling bag was connected to another storage bag using a silicon tube, and the gas in the sampling bag was transferred to the connected storage bag”? But how?

P6, L98-100: Is this the way to push the gas into another storage bag? However, the sampling bag shown in Fig.1 has only one port.

P7, L116: A reference is required for the effect of circadian rhythm on the cortisol change.

P8, L118-124: Please describe the odor assessment method more in detail to ensure the objectivity of this study. The four experts were recruited from out of author’s company (if from the company inside, please describe how to ensure the objectivity). Please demonstrate the process to characterize the odor in the bag by four experts.

P8, L132: What is the “each 1 L of skin gas”? I understand trace amount of skin gas was contained in 500 mL of nitrogen in the sampling bag. So, “skin gas” should be “gas in the sampling bag” or so. I am not sure about how to get 1 L of gas? What is “each”? The human skin gas used in this section were collected from who?

P8, L133-138: Please describe how the gas was trapped in the canister.

P8, L139-149: Who detected the gas by sniffing? One of the four experts?

P8, L150 Analysis 2: Please refer above comments on analysis 1 and revise them.

P11, L203: Please describe the model tension-stress odor more in detail. Is this a mixture of allyl mercaptan and dimethyl trisulfide? What is the solvent.

P14, L258-260: Please explore the recently published papers related to human skin gas analysis. The sulfur-containing compounds are usually found even at rest. So, I have a doubt on “none existed in the samples taken at baseline”. Please dispel my doubt by clearly showing the data obtained from the four experts, because the figure 4 seems very valuable.

P15, L276 Identifying characteristic odor: I respect author’s great efforts to identify the odor-causing gases. However, there is a big problem – control missing. I believe no peaks corresponding to allyl mercaptan and dimethyl trisulfide were found in the relax samples. So, please show the data (compare the chromatographs or so).

P16, L297-P17, L311: Based on the author’s deep experience, allyl mercaptan was successfully singled out as a causing substance. However, this section is very difficult to read. So, please sort the sentences including figure 8.

Figures captions: Please make the captions more informative. For example, which sample was used for analysis shown in Figures 6 and 8.

Reviewer #2:This study investigates relations between human subjective responses obtained by POMS and physiological responses such as gas evaporated from human skin surface of non-dominant hand, cortisol in saliva, and heart rate under a stressful state. In addition, it aims to create an odour model under stress/tense condition with which those who smell the odour are emotionally affected.

As a whole, the results in this study are shown in appropriate ways and are written in a comprehensive manner, suggesting that the main objective physiological response, skin gas, reflects subjective responses and correlates with other physiological indices reflecting sympathetic nervous system activity. Ethical consideration throughout the experiment is also appropriate. Sample size, experimental design, and analysis are properly administered as a whole. Thus this manuscript can be accepted after certain corrections and adding further explanations required to clarify the experimental procedure and its limitations. At the same time, the manuscript somewhat lacks balance of information volume in a way that some information on the experimental procedure is sufficient (such as the method to collect skin gas) and others insufficient. Especially descriptions on behavioural procedure is insufficient.

The following information includes the points that require further corrections or explanations are necessary.

Validity of the use of the term “psychological-” should be considered. The term has long been widely and inappropriately used in a wide variety of research papers to only represent human subjective responses, whereas psychology includes both subjective and objective aspects. Considering that various responses observed in animal psychology are typically non-verbal and thus must be measured only in objective manners, researchers no longer should not refer to psychological responses only as subjective. Furthermore, this study only employed POMS to evaluated participants’ subjective responses. The authors should alter the use of the term “psychological” into “subjective”, or at least define how the term is chosen to represent POMS data.

So called prescribed properties in this study should be described delicately since subjective measures should have been obtained with a more sufficient care. For example, the participants are set in the interview situation where they feel certain stress (l-232). Thus it is natural that they evaluate their subjective state in a emotionally-negative manner expecting what the experimenters anticipate. Thus how the properties are put into consideration in this study should at least be mentioned. Furthermore, the way how the experts evaluated the skin gas seems inappropriate in a sense the odours are evaluated without a blind method (l-119). If done in a blind manner, it should be noted, too.

Experimental design where the variable “stressful condition” is manipulated is not appropriate and this should also be mentioned in the manuscript. Admitting that the stressful condition is fairly controlled in a way the dependent variables are properly collected, the interview condition could have been more rigorously controlled. To be specific, a valid setting would be setting stressful (experimental condition) and non-stressful (control condition) interview/conversation conditions so that various activity levels are closer enough to each other to compare the difference in a more controlled way(l-251). Biking exercise is quantitatively and qualitatively different as an activity that it can not be rigorously compared. In that sense, cortisol level in saliva and heart rate are not compared in an ideal manner in this study. Such limitation of this study should at least be noted.

l-112

Time schedule of the experimental procudure as a whole should be illustrated in a more comprehensive way such as with a timeline figure. It is also unclear if the participants in each experiment are partly the same or different, which should also be noted.

l-113

Cortisol level can also be affected not only by the time of the day but also by the experimental surroundings which should easily affect sympathetic nervous system. Thus, the samples should have been collected on a different day at the same laboratory where the experiment was conducted. In addition, visual analogue scale (VAS) is recently used widely to evaluate various subjective responses in researches including studies on olfactory function. Furthermore, a Likert-style scale has fatal problems including anchoring effect that evaluators’s memory on the previous score affects following evaluations if done more than once in one experiment. It is partly because of the the problem that it can only be treated as a nominal or ordinal scale. Although such scales has long be used especially in the psychology field, the authors should at least explain disadvantages of such scales considering that there are accumulating numbers of studies claiming such questions.

l-193

The participants in this study are biased in age and gender. The authors should explain backgrounds of the distribution.

l-206

There are various subjective evaluation scales other than POMS. Although POMS is certainly one of the best measures and the authors properly explain the questionnaire in detail, the factors to be employed as compared with other famous measures should be simply noted. Here again, how the participants evaluated the scale (not VAS?) should be mentioned since they evaluated their state twice in the experiment.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1:No

Reviewer #2:No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool,https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS atgro.solp@serugif. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

  • PLoS One. 2022; 17(6): e0270457.
  • »
  • Author response to Decision Letter 0

2022; 17(6): e0270457.

Published online 2022 Jun 30. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0270457.r002

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

19 Apr 2022

Thank you very much for your detailed comments. I have italicized your comments and bolded my responses.

Reviewer #1: This is a very interesting paper which describes the change in skin odor under phycological stress, identification of skin gases causing the tension-stress odor and emotional effect of the odor to people by sniffing. The human skin gas has been inviting considerable attentions as a source of body odor, mosquito attractant, non-invasive medical biomarkers and so on. While a previous study showed the dermal emission of ammonia increased with physical and/or phycological stress, this study also found new skin gases causing the characteristic odor similar to sulfur-containing compounds. So, I believe this study potentially contains very valuable outcomes which merit publication. However, this manuscript has been poorly written with less objectivity. Please revise the manuscript addressing following points.

Thank you for your comments.

Below, I have provided answers and responses to some of the points you have raised.

First, the expression "the odor similar to sulfur-containing compounds " is too broad in meaning, so we changed the characteristic smell we found to a more specific expression: "the characteristic stir-fried leek-like odor".

P5, L76: I do not have any criticisms on this point, but simply want to know why the study subjects were focused on nonsmoking Japanese woman? Are there any effect of smoking on the human skin gas? Why woman only?

<Response P5, L76>

As the skin gas sampling in this study was performed on the hands, it was necessary to avoid tobacco odor contaminants found on the hands of smokers, so the target population was non-smokers. In addition, as body odor is known to differ between men and women, the study was initially restricted to women. An explanation has been added to the text.

P6, L94: I felt difficulty to catch the sentence that “Afterward, gas obtained from the sampling bags was recovered, placed in a storage bag (ANALYTIC-BARRIER™, GL Science Co., Ltd.) using connected silicone tubes.” Does this mean “Afterward, the sampling bag was connected to another storage bag using a silicon tube, and the gas in the sampling bag was transferred to the connected storage bag”? But how?

P6, L98-100: Is this the way to push the gas into another storage bag? However, the sampling bag shown in Fig.1 has only one port.

<Response P6, L94 & L98-100>

I apologize for the lack of clarity in my wording.

For skin gas collection, subjects washed their hands with unscented soap and the non-dominant hand was covered with a collection bag made of polyvinyl fluoride resin film (Tedlar®) with a one-way stopco*ck attached. First, the stopco*ck was opened and excess air was pushed out of the collection bag in which the hand was placed. Next, a bag (500 mL) filled with nitrogen gas was connected via a short silicone tube and the nitrogen-filled bag was pressed on to move the nitrogen into the collection bag. The empty nitrogen bag was then removed from the stopco*ck. After a certain amount of time (Fig. 2), a storage bag (ANALYTIC-BARRIER™, GL Sciences Corporation) was connected via a short silicone tube to the stopco*ck. The collection gas was transferred from the collection bag to the storage bag by pressing on the collection bag. This gas was used as the sample.

The collection method is as described above. These operating procedures are described in the revised manuscript.

P7, L116: A reference is required for the effect of circadian rhythm on the cortisol change.

<Response P7, L116:>

A paper on the diurnal variation of salivary cortisol has been added to the bibliography.

P8, L118-124: Please describe the odor assessment method more in detail to ensure the objectivity of this study. The four experts were recruited from out of author’s company (if from the company inside, please describe how to ensure the objectivity). Please demonstrate the process to characterize the odor in the bag by four experts.

<Response L118-124:>

The expert assessor was a person from within the company. The bags of odor were numbered and the assessors were not informed about the background of the samples. The raters shared a common perception of a distinctive, characteristic stir-fried leek-like odor and each rated the smell on a five-point scale from “no smell at all” to “strongly smelling” according to their own criteria. The operating instructions have been added.

P8, L132: What is the “each 1 L of skin gas”? I understand trace amount of skin gas was contained in 500 mL of nitrogen in the sampling bag. So, “skin gas” should be “gas in the sampling bag” or so. I am not sure about how to get 1 L of gas? What is “each”? The human skin gas used in this section were collected from who?

P8, L133-138: Please describe how the gas was trapped in the canister.

<Response P8, L132, L133-138:>

As you pointed out, firstly, the expression ' each 1 L of skin gas ' was incorrect. It has been corrected to the expression '1 L of sampled gas'. In this analysis, several skin gas samples obtained during the stress-inducing interviews were gathered by aspiration with a canister whose inner surface was deactivated and prepared with negative pressure. The operating instructions have been added.

P8, L139-149: Who detected the gas by sniffing? One of the four experts?

<Response P8, L139-149:>

In analysis 1, the odor was identified by one odor expert and one GC analyst who had learnt the odor characteristics from the odor expert.

In analysis 2, the odor was confirmed by two odor experts and a GC analyst.

This has been added to the text.

P8, L150 Analysis 2: Please refer above comments on analysis 1 and revise them.

<Response P8, L150:>

For analysis 2, we have also added a description of the odor evaluator and sample.

P11, L203: Please describe the model tension-stress odor more in detail. Is this a mixture of allyl mercaptan and dimethyl trisulfide? What is the solvent.

<Response P11, L203:>

Allyl mercaptan (AM) and dimethyl trisulfide (DMTS) were diluted with isopropyl alcohol to create five levels of concentration (AM: DMTS = (0.5: 2.5ppb), (1: 5ppb), (2: 10ppb), (5: 25ppb), (10: 50ppb)). 10 μL of each concentration was applied to a small piece of cotton. The cotton was placed under the subjects’ noses, which they sniffed.

I added this to the revised manuscript about this process.

P14, L258-260: Please explore the recently published papers related to human skin gas analysis. The sulfur-containing compounds are usually found even at rest. So, I have a doubt on “none existed in the samples taken at baseline”. Please dispel my doubt by clearly showing the data obtained from the four experts, because the figure 4 seems very valuable.

<Response P14, L258-260:>

The expression "the odor similar to sulfur-containing compounds " is too broad in meaning, so we changed the characteristic smell we found to a more specific expression: "the characteristic stir-fried leek-like odor". The first thing that caught our attention in this study was that differences were detected by a person's sense of smell.

This is the biggest differentiation from recent reports on the analysis of body odor, where differences were found using multivariate analysis and other techniques on the GC analyzed data. Of course, we recognize that sulfur compounds are also present in normal skin gases. In this study, the most significant feature is the presence or absence of a common characteristic odor component that can be perceived by humans: specifically, the characteristic stir-fried leek-like odor, and we have adopted a method to search for the main component of this odor. A description has been added before the analytical method.

P15, L276 Identifying characteristic odor: I respect author’s great efforts to identify the odor-causing gases. However, there is a big problem – control missing. I believe no peaks corresponding to allyl mercaptan and dimethyl trisulfide were found in the relax samples. So, please show the data (compare the chromatographs or so).

<Response P15, L276:>

In the gas chromatography analysis, no peak could be detected unless it was concentrated to a high concentration, but the human sense of smell could detect the presence or absence of this characteristic odor even if it was not concentrated.

The results (Fig. 6) show that this odor was not perceived in the samples taken during relaxation and exercise.

P16, L297-P17, L311: Based on the author’s deep experience, allyl mercaptan was successfully singled out as a causing substance. However, this section is very difficult to read. So, please sort the sentences including figure 8.

Figures captions: Please make the captions more informative. For example, which sample was used for analysis shown in Figures 6 and 8.

<Response P16, L297-P17, L311:>

The text has been reorganized and the captions amended.

Thank you very much for your detailed comments. I have italicized your comments and bolded my responses.

Reviewer #2: This study investigates relations between human subjective responses obtained by POMS and physiological responses such as gas evaporated from human skin surface of non-dominant hand, cortisol in saliva, and heart rate under a stressful state. In addition, it aims to create an odour model under stress/tense condition with which those who smell the odour are emotionally affected.

As a whole, the results in this study are shown in appropriate ways and are written in a comprehensive manner, suggesting that the main objective physiological response, skin gas, reflects subjective responses and correlates with other physiological indices reflecting sympathetic nervous system activity. Ethical consideration throughout the experiment is also appropriate. Sample size, experimental design, and analysis are properly administered as a whole. Thus this manuscript can be accepted after certain corrections and adding further explanations required to clarify the experimental procedure and its limitations. At the same time, the manuscript somewhat lacks balance of information volume in a way that some information on the experimental procedure is sufficient (such as the method to collect skin gas) and others insufficient. Especially descriptions on behavioural procedure is insufficient.

The following information includes the points that require further corrections or explanations are necessary.

Thank you for your comments. I apologize for the lack of organization in the text. I will provide an overall organized explanation before responding to each comment. We have also reorganized the abstract. First, the expression "the odor similar to sulfur-containing compounds " is too broad in meaning, so we changed the characteristic smell we found to a more specific expression: "the characteristic stir-fried leek-like odor".

The study consists of three points.

#1 The discovery of an odor like that of stir-fried leeks from the non-dominant hand of subjects in a state of stress, as supported by physiological indices

#2 Identification of the components of the characteristic leek odor

#3 By smelling the identified components, even in non-stressful situations, one can subjectively perceive tension and fatigue. In other words, it was shown that feelings can be propagated by smell.

Below, I have provided answers and responses to some of the points you raised.

Validity of the use of the term “psychological-” should be considered. The term has long been widely and inappropriately used in a wide variety of research papers to only represent human subjective responses, whereas psychology includes both subjective and objective aspects. Considering that various responses observed in animal psychology are typically non-verbal and thus must be measured only in objective manners, researchers no longer should not refer to psychological responses only as subjective. Furthermore, this study only employed POMS to evaluated participants’ subjective responses. The authors should alter the use of the term “psychological” into “subjective”, or at least define how the term is chosen to represent POMS data.

<Response :>

I understand your point and have changed 'psychological' to 'subjective'.

l-232

So called prescribed properties in this study should be described delicately since subjective measures should have been obtained with a more sufficient care. For example, the participants are set in the interview situation where they feel certain stress (l-232). Thus it is natural that they evaluate their subjective state in a emotionally-negative manner expecting what the experimenters anticipate. Thus how the properties are put into consideration in this study should at least be mentioned.

<Response: L-232>

The interview was, as you stated, a situation where we expected the participants to feel stressed. We showed that the interview produced a stress state, as we had expected, by checking fluctuations in the physiological indicators via electrocardiographic activity and salivary cortisol levels.

l-119

Furthermore, the way how the experts evaluated the skin gas seems inappropriate in a sense the odors are evaluated without a blind method (l-119). If done in a blind manner, it should be noted, too.

<Response: L-119>

The skin gas samples were numbered and the assessors were not informed of the background of the samples to help ensure a blind evaluation. The raters shared the perception of a distinctive odor like that of stir-fried leeks and rated its intensity on a five-point scale from “not smelling at all” to “strongly smelling”, according to their respective evaluation criteria. I have added the operating instructions.

Experimental design where the variable “stressful condition” is manipulated is not appropriate and this should also be mentioned in the manuscript. Admitting that the stressful condition is fairly controlled in a way the dependent variables are properly collected, the interview condition could have been more rigorously controlled. To be specific, a valid setting would be setting stressful (experimental condition) and non-stressful (control condition) interview/conversation conditions so that various activity levels are closer enough to each other to compare the difference in a more controlled way(l-251). Biking exercise is quantitatively and qualitatively different as an activity that it can not be rigorously compared. In that sense, cortisol level in saliva and heart rate are not compared in an ideal manner in this study. Such limitation of this study should at least be noted.

<Response :>

Thank you very much for the advice. In this exam, we created an environment where subjects were interviewed by people they did not know as a means of creating stress. Regardless of the contents of the interview, talking with a stranger can be stressful. For the relaxed state, we asked the subjects to read magazines that didn’t contain any stimulating topics.

We needed to prove that the characteristic leek-like odor produced during the interview was not simply due to increased sweating. Therefore, we had subjects exercise on a stationary bike as a means of increasing their heart rate and perspiration without increasing stress levels, and checked the odor of skin gases collected during this exercise.

l-112

Time schedule of the experimental procudure as a whole should be illustrated in a more comprehensive way such as with a timeline figure. It is also unclear if the participants in each experiment are partly the same or different, which should also be noted.

<Response l-112:>

A time schedule for the entire skin gas sampling experiment process during tension-induced interviews has been added.

Participants in the verification of the effect for subjective reaction of the model tension odor were some of the participants in the interview test and were asked to cooperate on another day.

l-113

Cortisol level can also be affected not only by the time of the day but also by the experimental surroundings which should easily affect sympathetic nervous system. Thus, the samples should have been collected on a different day at the same laboratory where the experiment was conducted. In addition, visual analogue scale (VAS) is recently used widely to evaluate various subjective responses in researches including studies on olfactory function. Furthermore, a Likert-style scale has fatal problems including anchoring effect that evaluators’s memory on the previous score affects following evaluations if done more than once in one experiment. It is partly because of the the problem that it can only be treated as a nominal or ordinal scale. Although such scales has long be used especially in the psychology field, the authors should at least explain disadvantages of such scales considering that there are accumulating numbers of studies claiming such questions.

<Response l-113:>

I understand your point about the timing of the saliva collection.

In this experiment, although the saliva from after the subjects read was collected during the same time of day the saliva was collected at home, the results were also included in the graph (fig. 4).In addition, I understand that the POMS Likert scale references the previous score, which causes an anchoring effect on the next evaluation. In the main text, we have added an explanation of its disadvantages.

l-193

The participants in this study are biased in age and gender. The authors should explain backgrounds of the distribution.

<Response l-193:>

As the skin gas sampling in this study was performed on the hands, it was necessary to avoid tobacco odor contaminants found on the hands of smokers, so the target population was non-smokers. In addition, as body odor is known to differ between men and women and with age, the study was initially restricted to women. I have added an explanation about this to the text.

l-206

There are various subjective evaluation scales other than POMS. Although POMS is certainly one of the best measures and the authors properly explain the questionnaire in detail, the factors to be employed as compared with other famous measures should be simply noted. Here again, how the participants evaluated the scale (not VAS?) should be mentioned since they evaluated their state twice in the experiment. <Response l-206:>

As you pointed out, there are various subjective evaluation scales. POMS was selected because the subjects of this study were healthy people and POMS has the ability to cover seven subjective emotional states. Subjects evaluate themselves twice in one experiment on a 5-point Likert scale. We described the disadvantages of answering using the Likert scale within the same study.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewer2(M_Katsuyama).docx

  • PLoS One. 2022; 17(6): e0270457.
  • »
  • Decision Letter 1

2022; 17(6): e0270457.

Published online 2022 Jun 30. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0270457.r003

Nobuyuki Sakai, Academic Editor

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

12 Jun 2022

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others

PONE-D-21-36189R1

Dear Dr. KATSUYAMA,

Thank you for your interesting manuscript. I have decided accepting your manuscript as an article in Plos One, but a reviewer had commented on your revision. You do not need to answer this comment any more, but the comment should be help your study in the future. Please keep thecomment described below.

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at gro.solp@gnillibrohtua.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact gro.solp@sserpeno.

Kind regards,

Nobuyuki Sakai, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1:All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2:All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1:Thank you very much for your great efforts to improve the manuscript. I think it is now acceptable for publication.

Reviewer #2:The authors have appropriately corrected the whole content, reflecting my suggestions. Thus no major revision will be necessary. The paper can be accepted with minor corrections. Yet the experimental design should be discussed a little further in the manuscript regarding its limitation in condition control since the authors have not fully responded to my suggestions (see below). I understood the authors's explanations but I believe they have not answered to my questions fully. How to control independent variables in a rigorous manner should be reconsidered closely again.

ref.

Experimental design where the variable “stressful condition” is manipulated is not appropriate and this should also be mentioned in the manuscript. Admitting that the stressful condition is fairly controlled in a way the dependent variables are properly collected, the interview condition could have been more rigorously controlled. To be specific, a valid setting would be setting stressful (experimental condition) and non-stressful (control condition) interview/conversation conditions so that various activity levels are closer enough to each other to compare the difference in a more controlled way(l-251). Biking exercise is quantitatively and qualitatively different as an activity that it can not be rigorously compared. In that sense, cortisol level in saliva and heart rate are not compared in an ideal manner in this study. Such limitation of this study should at least be noted.

<response>

Thank you very much for the advice. In this exam, we created an environment where subjects were interviewed by people they did not know as a means of creating stress. Regardless of the contents of the interview, talking with a stranger can be stressful. For the relaxed state, we asked the subjects to read magazines that didn’t contain any stimulating topics. We needed to prove that the characteristic leek-like odor produced during the interview was not simply due to increased sweating. Therefore, we had subjects exercise on a stationary bike as a means of increasing their heart rate and perspiration without increasing stress levels, and checked the odor of skin gases collected during this exercise.</response>

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1:No

Reviewer #2:No

**********

  • PLoS One. 2022; 17(6): e0270457.
  • »
  • Acceptance letter

2022; 17(6): e0270457.

Published online 2022 Jun 30. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0270457.r004

Nobuyuki Sakai, Academic Editor

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

22 Jun 2022

PONE-D-21-36189R1

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others

Dear Dr. Katsuyama:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact gro.solp@sserpeno.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at gro.solp@enosolp.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Nobuyuki Sakai

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others (2024)

FAQs

How emotional changes affect skin odor and its impact on others? ›

Our findings indicate that humans reproducibly emit discernible odors through the skin containing allyl mercaptan and dimethyl trisulfide during interviews that induce psychological tension. Furthermore, we found that when other people smell this odor, they undergo subjective tension, confusion, and tiredness.

Does emotion affect body odor? ›

A growing body of research suggests that emotional chemosignals in others' body odor (BO), particularly those sampled during fearful states, enhance emotional face perception in conscious and preconscious stages. For instance, emotional faces access visual awareness faster when presented with others' fear BOs.

What is the fear of smelling bad to others? ›

The fear of smelling bad, also known as bromidrophobia, is a relatively common fear for many people. It can lead to an excessive concern with being clean and avoiding smells, and sometimes it can lead to severe symptoms of anxiety that interfere with a person's ability to function normally in their daily life.

What is the connection between smell and emotions? ›

Odors take a direct route to the limbic system, including the amygdala and the hippocampus, the regions related to emotion and memory. “The olfactory signals very quickly get to the limbic system,” Murthy said.

Why can't I smell myself but others can? ›

Although we can indeed smell our own odors — a quick sniff of the underarm will bear this out — over time, we become desensitized to our particular scent, said Hiroaki Matsunami, a molecular neurobiologist at Duke University.

Can emotional stress cause body odor? ›

Sweating and body odor are common when you exercise or you're too warm. They're also common when you're feeling nervous, anxious or stressed. Unusual changes in sweating — either too much (hyperhidrosis) or too little (anhidrosis) — can be cause for concern. Changes in body odor also may signal a health problem.

Can mental health affect body odor? ›

It doesn't happen to everyone, but some people find that their body odor appears to significantly change when they have anxiety. This change may be subtle or may occur to a degree that they feel more anxious in social situations due to fear of developing a noticeable odor.

Why can I smell myself through my pants? ›

Limiting frequent bathing can lead to dirt, sweat and dead skin cells build up leading to Your clothes have a strong smell. You can reduce these effects by showering regularly and washing with mild soap and warm water. Sweating in the groin area can attract fungi and bacteria that lead to bad odors.

Can others smell my discharge? ›

The truth is that when everything's normal, no one else smells any odors from a girl's vagin*. If a girl has an infection like bacterial vaginosis, she may notice a fishy smell. But even then, it's unlikely that people around her will notice it.

What is the emotional response to odors? ›

Odors can also elicit basic emotions, like those proposed by Ekman14: fear, happiness, disgust, anger, surprise, and sadness. However, the majority of studies indicate that basic emotions evoked by odors are predominantly related to happiness and disgust as two opposite sides of a continuum.

What does psychology say about scent? ›

The sense of smell is closely linked with memory, probably more so than any of our other senses. Those with full olfactory function may be able to think of smells that evoke particular memories; the scent of an orchard in blossom conjuring up recollections of a childhood picnic, for example.

What are the psychological effects of odor? ›

Any odor that for a given individual evokes a happy autobiographical memory has the potential to increase positive emotions, decrease negative moods, disrupt cravings, lower stress and decrease inflammatory immune responses, and thereby have a generally beneficial effect on psychological and physiological wellbeing.

What is stinky finger syndrome? ›

Abstract. Many accounts refer to insertion of finger into anus mostly for gratification from stimulation of prostate gland, but index case Mr. M. continued doing this to get rid of constipation that eventually led to feelings of guilt, stinky fingers, not able to defecate normally, and dysphoric emotions.

What would anxiety smell like? ›

Common Phantom Smells Anxiety Symptom Descriptions

This unusual smell is not related to anything you've eaten or been exposed to. Common descriptions of the smell include, “tinny,” “metallic,” “ammonia-like,” “blood-like,” “sour,” “bitter,” “acidy,” “vinegary,” “rotting-like,” and repugnant, to name a few.

Why do some people smell bad? ›

Body odor is caused by a mix of bacteria and sweat on your skin. Your body odor can change due to hormones, the food you eat, infection, medications or underlying conditions like diabetes. Prescription-strength antiperspirants or medications may help.

What affects body odor the most? ›

Your body odor can change due to hormones, the food you eat, infection, medications or underlying conditions like diabetes. Prescription-strength antiperspirants or medications may help.

How your personality affects the way you smell? ›

Odor and emotions

For instance, people who are neurotic may sweat more when they experience stress, which would modify the bacteria in their underarms and make them smell different, the researchers said. Personality traits may also be linked with the secretion of hormones that could alter a persons' scent.

What are the 10 causes of body odour? ›

Causes of body odour
  • being overweight.
  • consuming rich or spicy food and drink – such as garlic, spices and alcohol.
  • some types of medication – such as antidepressants.
  • certain medical conditions – a fruity smell can sometimes be a sign of diabetes, while a bleach-like smell may suggest liver or kidney disease.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Dong Thiel

Last Updated:

Views: 5562

Rating: 4.9 / 5 (79 voted)

Reviews: 94% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Dong Thiel

Birthday: 2001-07-14

Address: 2865 Kasha Unions, West Corrinne, AK 05708-1071

Phone: +3512198379449

Job: Design Planner

Hobby: Graffiti, Foreign language learning, Gambling, Metalworking, Rowing, Sculling, Sewing

Introduction: My name is Dong Thiel, I am a brainy, happy, tasty, lively, splendid, talented, cooperative person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.